Anti-mathemagenic web activity

There is a concept related to learning and studying that I have carried over from my grad school days. E. Rothkopf proposed a concept he called a “mathemagenic” activity. I remember mathemagenic to translate as giving birth to knowledge or something similar. It was the idea that an external activity could encourage productive cognitive behaviors in some learners and at the time I remember it being associated with adjunct or interspersed questions. In other words, attempting to answer questions can engage students in thinking behaviors they might not generate on their own. I suppose there are similar concepts, e.g., generative activity, but I continue to use the original term rather than adopt newer, but similar terms.

Anyway, I like the idea that external activities can encourage productive internal behaviors. I assume this assumption is what encourages suggested study strategies and learning tasks. As I have used the concept in my own teaching, I began to consider whether some external activities could be anti-mathemagenic. In other words, could certain activities limit learning. My original example originated from thinking about my own behavior. In my example, highlighting is the external behavior. Sometimes it seemed when I was feeling lazy I would identify important ideas while reading that I did not fully understand, but I would highlight this material assuming I would return and think more carefully later. Of course, if I failed to return to review, I would be worse off for deferring the work of understanding.

I have read Carr and other critics of Internet information processing and I have begun wondering whether certain patterns of Internet use are anti-mathemagenic. The key here would be if some less productive behavior were substituted for more productive behavior.  I sometimes find myself using Instapaper in a way that could be mathemagenic. Instead of fully reading something I identify as potentially valuable, I send things to instapaper and I assume I will read carefully later. I think moving from blogging to micro-blogging would fit. Blogging is work. It takes time and thought. Microblogging (Twitter) is easy in comparison. Twitter may be an effective way to offer links to others, but it offers little in encouraging personal thinking.

Mathemagenic and anti-mathemagenic activities are difficult to spot in someone else. You can’t see internal behaviors. However, I am guessing if what you are doing is easy or quick little cognitive activity is involved.

Loading

Am I getting credit for this?

Cindy recently relayed to me a post from the IT forum regarding blogging (and I assume other forms of online expression) and whether such activity should count for anything among those of us who are college academics. This got me thinking. I have blogged since 2003 and authored content for pubic consumption before that. Sometimes I did the latter because of funding I had secured (I was lucky enough to be in on a Technology Challenge grant many years ago), but mostly I wrote and still write as a hobby. Perhaps I should now argue for compensation. I also serve as a department-level administrator and I deal with contracts and expectations on a daily basis. I have listened to many arguments related to what faculty members would like to receive credit for doing. My point? I can imagine myself dealing with this topic from several perspectives.

First, here are some of the links referenced on the listserv and on linked blogs:
Hastac
Virtual School Meanderings

My reaction as blogger:
If I were to attempt to make a case for my online content as part of me job, I think I would argue that it would be part of my service or perhaps part of my teaching (as professional development). I think some of the things I have read (listed above and comments to those posts) reach the same conclusion, but then note that this does not really count for much. I work (except for my administrative responsibilities) in what is pretty much a 60, 30, 10 model (teaching, research, service). Most of the evaluation for teaching relates to what I do with students and very little to my own efforts at professional development. I don’t know that I even bother to list professional development activities (what I read, time spent at conferences, etc.) when I list my annual activities. The 10% for service also gets broken down and variability from one faculty member to another is some fraction of 10% amounts to very little difference in the annual merit evaluation and certainly little toward major decisions (tenure, promotion).

I do not regard my activity as research and I have reviewed very little online that I feel should qualify. I am an educational researcher by profession and my research activity is assumed to be based on data that I collect. I publish formal papers based on these data and my analysis. Writing in reaction to what I read elsewhere is just my opinion and not research. My opinion may be worth something because of my personal experiences, but offering such opinions would be public scholarship as teaching or service and not research.

My reaction as administrator:
When we hire individuals we make a good faith effort to explain the type of program we are and the type of activities we expect of those we hire. Certainly, this is what we expect of individuals in evaluating them for tenure. If individuals decide they would like to spend time in creative ways after tenure, we encourage such behavior (I think the faculty handbook says 20% of effort) but what one does with this 20% does not guarantee anything when it comes to merit. I guess my point is that it is possible that offering “information” to the public in the form of blogs or other unmoderated content could be of value if an institution say offering such information to be part of the institutional mission. I think you ask this question before accepting a contract and not argue about expectations later.

If a blogger (or writer) can make some money I think we accept this as part of the 20% allowed for such activities.

Powered by ScribeFire.

Loading

Flashcards

I have an assignment in my Educational Psychology class that asks each student to try and then evaluate a study strategy. I offer a list with some options and allow students to add their own. Past experience has demonstrated that the students gravitate toward a limited number of options. I now require students to register their selection with me and I cut off an option after it has been selected by 10 students. I want to create a situation in which we have some different options to discuss.

The most popular choice this semester was note cards (flash cards). So much for trying something new. It has always struck me as a little sad that advanced students seem focused on memorization. Perhaps they see the challenges of exams differently than I do. I think I am asking them to demonstrate or apply, but they see the most immediate challenge as recognizing key terms in the questions. Perhaps they feel that once they can translate any unique terminology, the rest tends to be easy. So, I have mixed feelings about this post. Here, I offer a suggestion for a “better” flashcard system. Clearly knowing some things is necessary, but we hope students make the investment to do more.

Handheld devices (phones, iPod, iPad) offer a convenient technology by which improvements to note card flashcards might be accomplished. I see I generated a post in 2008 describing flashcard apps for the iPod Touch. Cindy has been exploring newer flashcard systems for the iPad so I purchased a few myself and spent some time during the football game exploring. My favorite ended up being Flashcard Deluxe. I purchased the version I used to generate the screen captures, but there is a lite version you might explore.

Technology offers the opportunity to improve the traditional flash card experience in several ways. The advantages I describe are available in my recommended app, but may not be in alternatives.
1) A device-based system can collect data on performance. With Flashcard Deluxe, the student determines whether a response is correct or not (actually a three level system is available – strong answer, moderate answer, poor answer) and this delineation can be used to change the probabilities of seeing an item again.
You swipe a different direction to indicate which best represents your answer. Items can also be categorized. The self-scoring system also allows the question types to be varied – you could use multiple choice, but why not use open ended questions. Just creating the questions and responses is likely a valuable learning experience.
2) A device-based system allows multimedia. My daughters were heavy flashcard users because of the types of content they studied to become a physical therapist and an occupational therapist. Cindy did get the youngest to use a technology-based system. I don’t know if they would have sketched representations of the things they studied or not. How do you study musculature and the skeletal system with terms and definitions?
3) A digital system allows the accumulation and sharing of content. Flashcard Deluxe allows sets of flashcards to be downloaded from Quizlet and from a collection accumulated by OrangeorApple (the company responsible for Flashcard Deluxe). A student or group of students could create and share study aids.

A couple of screen captures:

This is the card that allows the creation of a given flashcard. There are three sides – a question, the answer, and other info. You do not have to use all three. In the following image, you can probably figure out that the question is really an image (the picture), the second side is the name of the organism, and the third side provides some additional information. I was a bit lazy in generating this example. You can include a lot of content on each side.

So, this would be the “question” side of the card.

This would be the third side (in this case, the deck was intended to be a collection of macroinvertebrates that indicate good or poor water quality).


Powered by ScribeFire.

Loading

Why proven ideas are not used

I don’t focus on the research literature on this site, but I want to make an exception. I encourage those of you interested in educational research make the effort to read the article by Rohrer & Pahler in the June/July issue of Educational Researcher (2010, 39(5), 406-412).

The article argues that researchers have made a number of concrete suggestions that would improve student learning and that these suggestions are often ignored. The three examples offered include – learning through testing (increasing retrieving), spacing of practice, and interleaving. These examples were selected because the basis for the suggestion are quite solid AND because the suggestions are about studying differently rather than studying more.

Perhaps the most interesting part of the review was a speculative addition at the end of the presentation which asked the question “Why are inferior strategies so popular?” It is this focus I think should receive wider consideration because it may extend to many current topics in education. The authors pose the question in an interesting fashion. Since some of these suggestions could have developed as common practice simply as a function of student trial and error, why do students typically spend their time in inferior strategies? Remember these proposals are about a different way of doing things and not about the expectation that students spend more time. The authors suggest that these strategies tend to produce a higher error rate during study which may be more discouraging for students. It appears students prefer passive strategies because there is less challenge to their illusion of understanding. I wonder. Perhaps students do understand that a technique is less effective, but still persist because it is what they know and it is easier.

Powered by ScribeFire.

Loading

Multi-Taskers? Not really.

I had my educational psychology class complete a short in-class writing assignment related to the topic of working memory and multi-tasking. I originally had no specific motive beyond getting them to apply some of these ideas to their own experiences and so I asked them to write about listening to music while they studied. There were some specific prompts, but this was the general topic. I have read so many who claim younger folks are multi-taskers and prefer this or that environment for learning that I guess I was somewhat surprised with how the students responded.

After reading the responses, I created a simple way to organize what I thought each student had said and I came up with this distribution. If one combines the students who say they listen to no music with those who say they use music to mask out other sounds, it would be pretty difficult to argue these students must multi-task.

I did encounter some interesting comments regarding musical tastes (what works while studying) and when music is helpful and when not. Avoid popular music with lyrics (one student claimed to listen to game tracks). Music while writing and working problem sets (math, chemistry) seems workable, but do not try to listen to music while reading.

Of course, there were some who say they listen to music constantly, but the point is these individuals were not typical. Sometimes I wonder about the methodology used by those who attempt to make the case that learners are changing and educators need to take notice and make adjustments. What I did was quite informal as far as research goes and not really designed to test formal hypotheses, but the typical student in this group is not a multi-tasker.

Loading

Shutterfly Classroom

I have been wanting to explore Shutterfly for some time, but because I pay for a Flickr account and have spent considerable time with Picasa, it seemed like just one more version of what I have been recommending. In the back of mind what I knew was unique about Shutterfly was that while it was a photo sharing site it also offered a way to offer general web content. This struck me as an important distinction.

A few days ago I came across this strange post on MarketWatch (strange because MarketWatch is not exactly the place I search for ideas for the classroom). The business oriented post that includes the present stock market value of Shutterfly explained Shutterfly’s effort to offer classroom web sites. The article explains that parents would like to see images from the classroom and from student field trips, but providing such images is difficult because of security concerns. So, Shutterfly has offered a version of the tool allowing the creation of web sites to meet this and other classroom to home communication needs. The classroom web sites are by invitation only which is the way the teacher or a parent volunteer controls access. While Shutterfly’s main business model is printing user photographs in various formats (images, calendars, photo books) and photos are clearly what Shutterfly does well, the template and modular-based approach makes it relatively easy to create an impressive site.

Main web page

Shutterfly album

Powered by ScribeFire.

Loading