Learners experience information events as primary source content

Every once in a while, I come up with something I think is clever. The challenge at this point is to convince others that my insight is clever and usually to try to get them to understand my insight.

Here is my new conceptual proposal – to an effective learner all content they encounter is really experienced as a primary source. The inability to experience content in this way limits understanding and eventual application. This inability can be due to poor aptitude or poor attitude. In practice, these problems can be interrelated and mutually inflammatory. Additional learning experiences are necessary to address either problem.

The context for this observation was my thinking about an upcoming discussion of direct vs. constructivist models of instruction. I decided that making this traditional distinction is flawed and actually violates what constructivism means when constructivism is used as a description of learning. Constructivism as a description of cognitive activity implies that each learner engages in unique, knowledge building activities to make sense of experiences in the context of what a learner already knows. Simulations are experiences. Daily observations of life events are experiences. Reading a book is an experience. Listening to a presentation is an experience. Constructivism is about what the learner does with external experiences and not some classification of these external experiences.

The notion of primary source (as used say by a historian) takes a similar perspective. The inputs  (data of some type) are subjected to processing in an effort to achieve meaning.The distinction between a primary and a secondary source is really most accurate for the individual who has generated the secondary source. Even a secondary source as traditionally described (say a textbook) has to be treated as a primary source by the effective learner. Processing is required for understanding.

For anyone who processes an input, flaws may arise from lack of skill or motivation. Lack of useful existing knowledge may limit integration. Lack of motivation may limit the willingness to search for relevant existing knowledge, to add new knowledge to benefit understanding, or to test alternative interpretations.

I get tired of the strange description that those of us who were educated by reading books and listening to presentations had knowledge dumped into our heads. It seems possible that we accepted this as an input (attitude) and understood our job was to think about such inputs. Additional inputs may have been necessary and helpful when personal processing was not initially sufficient, but whether learning resulted from the initial or the secondary inputs, the personal processing was what ultimately determined whether we understood or not.

Loading

Maybe everyone is right – sorta

I am writing and attempting to make my way between the conflicting views on instructional strategies. The controversy between those who support direct instruction and those who support authentic, problem-based learning activities has always troubled me. Both seem to make sound arguments and those supporting direct instruction point to multiple meta-analyses demonstrating the superiority of greater structure. We want to offer teachers sound advice and we would feel more comfortable if we could come to some personal way to resolve what seem to be inconsistent positions.

Strange as it may seem my person way of thinking about this situation has been informed by a topic I cover when teaching Introduction to Psychology. There was an interesting and long term controversy within the field of perception that concerned the mechanism by which we experience color. One position, the trichromatic theory of color perception, proposes that we have three types of color receptors differentially sensitive to red, green, and blue. The ratio of neural activity generated by the differential stimulation of these three types of receptors is thought to generate what we perceive as color. There is good evidence for this position. There are different patterns of color blindness, but the specific colors that cannot be identified by those with different types of color blindness can be accounted for by assuming that one or more of the three types of receptors are missing. The competing theory, the opponent process theory, assumes that color perception is the result of receptors that are sensitive to a continua – white to black, red to green, and yellow to blue. The experience of a negative afterimage, the color you see after staring at a color for some time and then looking at a white background, offers convincing evidence for the opponent process explanation. On some occasion, you may have experienced a demonstration in which you stared at an American flag with green and blue stripes and black stars against a yellow background and then viewed the colors of the actual flag when looking at a pure white background.

What is challenging about this controversy and what may be germane to other disputes is that the competing explanations are each supported by convincing evidence. Each side holds on to its position and unique evidence not really addressing the evidence that supports the other perspective. With color vision, eventually a more general theory was offered to reconcile these competing theories. It was proposed that these mechanisms were both valid but operated in stages. The trichromatic theory appeared to apply to the cones and the opponent process theory at a latter point as information moved from the eye to the brain. It turned out both explanations made a contribution and both were necessary. A Nobel award was involved.

I think the same model may be applied to experiential and direction experiences – maybe both are useful. Sometimes, a learner has little personal experience and some experiences might be the way to begin. Sometimes, learners have many experiences but no model for understanding them. Perhaps this is when direct instruction makes sense. It may be the mix and the order that we should be discussing rather than blasting away with a model takes all approach.

Loading

Reality Check

I have decided that my formal academic training is somewhat of an obstacle. It functions somewhat like a conscience. It is that little voice in the back of my head reminding me that it is inappropriate to be a promoter without having solid evidence to back claims I may find advantageous to advocate. This is probably the one issue that troubles me the most when I read many blogs related to my personal interests. I don’t mind bloggers promoting ideas associated with personal profit motives (e.g., books, speaking or consultant fees) because I do believe individuals with good ideas should be compensated for their knowledge and skills. However, those in such situations do have a responsibility to seriously consider the evidence for and against the positions they accept fees for promoting. In my world view, this is about science and not promotion.

Many participatory or constructivist positions on education (this covers a great deal of ground) are contrasting themselves with something. This alternative is described in a variety of ways – the traditional approach, the instructivist approach, the lecture approach, instructor centered, etc. – and as a hold over from another time requiring different skills (an assembly line mentality). Clearly, occupations have changed and require different skills or at least a change in the skills that are emphasized. Clearly, information can often be conveniently accessed from external sources and need not be stored in personal memory. The issue is not so much that performance in changing times requires more sophisticiation and a different set of skills, but how best to develop these skills. There are many proposals that essentially suggest that new skills require different learning experiences and some of these proposals have come from me. What that little voice in my head keeps reminding me is that I should be able to offer sound emprical support when I make such claims.

This concern comes and goes with me. At present the topic is more salient because I am teaching a graduate educational psyçhology course and engaging students in the consideration of constructivism. I have always felt this unease when presenting/discussing this topic noting that the “research base” in the reading assignments I use seem weak in contrast to the references available for other topics. My personal preparation for these discussions also engages me with challenging positions taken by very well respected scholars (a partial list appears at the end of this entry). Why is it that the blogs I read seem unaware of these contrary positions and seem unable to respond in kind by offering empirical support for the positions they advocate?

As if by magic, one blog I follow (EdTechDev) has posted an entry that at least seems to acknowledge my concern. D Holton seems to be thinking along similar lines in recognizing this unacknowledged challenge.

I sometimes wish there would be an opportunity to get these folks together in the same room and make them deal with each other. One problem with reading what people have to say is that individuals are free to ignore core issues and continue to harp on personal perspectives. If the blogosphere is to move causes forward by “churning ideas”, part of the process must require a more direct approach to considering the data presented by contrasting positions. So, I guess this is a challenge – how about a little more effort directed toward reading what the position being put down has to offer and a commitment to offer up a strong refutation (e.g., Hake).

– – – –

Chall, J. (2000). The academic achievement challenge: What really works in classrooms. New York: Guilford Press.

Kirschner, P.A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R.E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work. Educational Psychologist, 41, 75-86.

Lesgold, A. (2001). The nature and methods of learning by doing. American Psychologist, 56(11), 964-973.

Mayer, R. (2001). Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery? The case for guided methods of instruction. American Psychologist, 59, 14-19.

Loading